0
Article |

Rapid Eye Movement Sleep Percentage in Children With Autism Compared With Children With Developmental Delay and Typical Development FREE

Ashura Williams Buckley, MD; Alcibiades J. Rodriguez, MD; Kaitlin Jennison, BA; Jack Buckley, PhD; Audrey Thurm, PhD; Susumu Sato, MD; Susan Swedo, MD
[+] Author Affiliations

Author Affiliations: National Institute of Mental Health (Drs A. W. Buckley, Thurm, and Swedo and Ms Jennison) and National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (Dr Sato), Bethesda, Maryland; and New York University, New York (Drs A. W. Buckley, Rodriguez, and J. Buckley).


Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2010;164(11):1032-1037. doi:10.1001/archpediatrics.2010.202.
Text Size: A A A
Published online

Objective  To compare objective polysomnographic parameters between 3 cohorts: children with autism, typical development, and developmental delay without autism.

Design  Overnight polysomnographic recordings were scored for sleep architecture according to American Academy of Sleep Medicine criteria by a board-certified sleep medicine specialist blind to diagnosis for studies collected between July 2006 and September 2009.

Setting  Subjects were evaluated in the pediatric ward in the Clinical Research Center of the National Institutes of Health.

Participants  First 60 consecutive children with autism, 15 with typical development, and 13 with developmental delay matched for nonverbal IQ to the autism group, ranging in age from 2 to 13 years, selected without regard to the presence or absence of sleep problem behavior.

Main Outcome Measures  Total sleep time, latencies to non–rapid eye movement (REM) and REM sleep, and percentages of total sleep time for stages 1 and 2 sleep, slow-wave sleep, and REM sleep.

Results  There were no differences between the typical vs developmental delay groups. Comparison of children with autism vs typical children revealed shorter total sleep time (P = .004), greater slow-wave sleep percentage (P = .001), and much smaller REM sleep percentage (14.5% vs 22.6%; P < .001). Comparison of children with autism vs children with developmental delay revealed shorter total sleep time (P = .001), greater stage 1 sleep percentage (P < .001), greater slow-wave sleep percentage (P < .001), and much less REM sleep percentage (14.5% v 25%; P < .001).

Conclusion  A relative deficiency of REM sleep may indicate an abnormality in neural organization in young children with autism that is not directly associated with or related to inherent intellectual disability but may serve as a window into understanding core neurotransmitter abnormalities unique to this disorder.

Figures in this Article

Autism is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by abnormalities in social interaction and communication and by the presence of repetitive behaviors.1 Abnormalities in gross brain structure, neuronal growth patterns, and neurotransmitter profiles have all been preliminarily explored with little consensus on findings as they relate to pathophysiology.2 Consistent behavioral disturbances may offer insights into core physiologic or anatomic abnormalities and may help to elucidate biological signatures or endophenotypes. Disturbances in sleep are well-known correlates of autism3 and sleep disturbances in children with autism are a major clinical concern for caretakers and clinicians. The prevalence has been estimated to be between 44% and 83% for this population.4 Irregular sleep, frequent night awakenings, prolonged awakenings, and hypersomnia alternating with severely reduced sleep times are the most often reported abnormalities.5 Most of the previous studies addressing sleep in autism have relied on parent-report measures alone69 or in conjunction with actigraphic recordings,8,10 which may allow for the tracking of overall sleep-wake cycles more objectively but does not inform on specific sleep architecture, such as relative ratios of rapid eye movement (REM) and non-REM sleep. Although there is clearly a great need for therapeutic interventions that will alleviate these sleep disturbances, progress toward developing such therapies has been stymied by a lack of useful information regarding the underlying neurobiological abnormality.11

Markers of sleep architecture and sleep state can be objectively and noninvasively measured and referenced to developmental norms. Polysomnography is a reliable, noninvasive tool used to study the basic mechanisms of sleep and has proven useful to neuropsychiatric medicine by serving to identify trait markers for diseases such as narcolepsy and depression.12 Previous studies in people with autism have identified various abnormalities in REM sleep, including immature organization, decreased quantity, abnormal twitches, undifferentiated sleep, and REM sleep behavior disorder, which is characterized by the absence of the muscle atonia that is normal during REM sleep.1316 There have been relatively few exclusively pediatric polysomnographic studies of autism to date. Existing studies have consistently confirmed various abnormalities of sleep but are often difficult to compare because of dissimilar exclusion and inclusion criteria, different age groups, and small number of patients.17,18

Given the recent progress in delineating the neurobiology of sleep, investigating putatively abnormal sleep architecture (as it may reflect abnormal neurotransmission) in a large, well-defined, young cohort of children with autism offers the opportunity of identifying trait markers in autism that may be rapidly linkable to emerging genetic findings. Even if such an ambitious goal is not realized, the information gained could have immediate benefits in terms of developing more targeted therapies for the sleep disturbances of autism.

SUBJECTS

Our institutional review board approved this study and the cohort presented is a subset from 2 ongoing National Institute of Mental Health investigations, a small clinical trial of minocycline and a larger phenomenologic investigation of the medical and clinical subtypes of autism. Children were recruited to participate in the studies without regard to the presence or absence of sleep problem behavior. This report includes the first 60 children between the ages of 2 and 13 years to be given a research diagnosis of autism (see later), complete an overnight polysomnogram at the National Institute of Health Clinical Research Center, and have a technically complete study (studies where eye leads were lost were not included). Autism symptoms were assessed using the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule,19 a clinician-administered structured play interview designed to elicit behaviors relevant to a diagnosis of autism, and the Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised,20 a semistructured parent interview concerning all domains of impairment in autism spectrum disorders. The developmentally delayed group was ascertained for matching on developmental and adaptive capacity to the autism group. Criteria for this group included a non–autism spectrum disorder clinical judgment based on administration of the Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised and Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule. Of the first 15 children with developmental delay to complete an overnight polysomnogram, 13 (aged 2-7 years) had technically complete studies and are included in this data set. The typical control group was recruited at a younger age to better match maturational and neurodevelopment levels with the delays seen in the autism group and consists of the first 15 children (aged 1-5 years) to complete an overnight study. Each parent or guardian also completed the Children's Sleep Habits Questionnaire.21 Medication histories were obtained from all participants to evaluate potential effects of medication on sleep architecture.

POLYSOMNOGRAM

Children were admitted for a continuous overnight recording that included a referential, 21-lead electroencephalogram montage, electro-oculogram, electrocardiogram, and surface electromyogram (chin, anterior tibialis). Respiratory parameters were not measured. Lights out approximated child's actual bedtime. All recordings were videotaped and ended at a median wake up time of 7 AM for all groups. The clinical readings were provided by the same neurophysiologist (S.S.) using National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke electroencephalogram laboratory standards for sleep architecture; these early reports of decreased REM sleep percentage among the children with autism were the impetus for this systemic investigation. A second, blinded reading for sleep architecture and leg movements was done using Grass telefactor software (Grass Technologies, West Warwick, Rhode Island) by a different neurologist, board certified in neurology, neurophysiology, and sleep medicine (A.J.R.). Wake/sleep was subdivided into 30-second epochs and scored according to the guidelines contained in The AASM [American Academy of Sleep Medicine] Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events.22 The following sleep variables were calculated: total sleep time (the entire sleep period minus the time spent in wakefulness after sleep onset), sleep efficiency index (total sleep time divided by time in bed × 100), the minutes spent in each sleep stage (stage 1, stage 2, stage 3 or slow-wave sleep, and REM sleep), the percentage of each stage relative to total sleep time, the latency to sleep onset (measured from lights out to the first epoch of stage 1), and the latency to REM sleep (measured from the first epoch of stage 1 to the first epoch of REM sleep).

All participants were allowed to continue taking any prescribed medications during the hospitalization. One child in the typical group was taking diphenhydramine and cetirizine on the night of the study. Four children in the development delay group reported taking a medication on the night of the study that could potentially affect sleep architecture: clobazam (1), somatostatin (1), cetirizine (1), and montelukast (1). In the autism group, 11 children took the following medications on the night of the study: clonidine alone (2); risperidone alone (2); melatonin alone (1); carbamazepine and fluoxetine (1); melatonin and quetiapine (1); melatonin, fluvoxamine, oxcarbazepine, and trazodone (1); topiramate and clonazepam (1); cetirizine (1); and valproate (1).

STATISTICAL ANALYSIS

Descriptive statistics, including medians and interquartile ranges, were computed for all sleep parameters and additional measures (age, nonverbal ratio IQ). The normality of distributions of the sleep parameters was assessed using the Shapiro-Francia W′ test, revealing significant nonnormality in 4 of the 8 measures (wakefulness after sleep onset, sleep efficiency, stage 1 sleep percentage, and latency to REM sleep; P < .001). Sample variances for all parameters were compared between groups using the Brown and Forsythe robust test using the sample median as the measure of location; no variances were found to be statistically significantly different at the P = .01 level. Pairwise differences between groups (autism, typical, and developmentally delayed) on all measures were thus evaluated by comparing medians to account for nonnormality, and statistical significance was assessed via the Mann-Whitney U test. Tests were 2-sided, and a P value of .01 was considered to indicate statistical significance. In addition, 99% confidence intervals for median differences were computed based on the Hodges-Lehmann statistic.23 All calculations were generated using Stata statistical analysis software, version 10.1 (StataCorp, College Station, Texas).

Means, standard deviations, and ranges of the observed data are presented in Table 1. Table 2 presents medians, interquartile ranges, and the results of Mann-Whitney tests and Hodges-Lehmann confidence intervals. As Table 2 shows, compared with the typically developing group, the subjects with autism showed a significantly shorter total sleep time (P = .004), a significantly higher percentage of stage 3 sleep (P = .001), and a significantly lower percentage of REM sleep (P < .001).

Table Graphic Jump LocationTable 2. Children With Autism Have Shorter Total Sleep Time, Increased SWS, and Relative REM Sleep Deficiency

Compared with the developmental delay group, the subjects with autism showed a significantly shorter total sleep time (P = .001), a significantly longer stage 1 sleep (P < .001), a significantly higher percentage of stage 3 sleep (P < .001), and a significantly lower percentage of REM sleep (P < .001).

There were no statistically significant differences between the typical group and the developmental delay group on any sleep measurements. Notably, sleep efficiencies and minutes awake after sleep onset did not differ between groups.

The median wake time in the clinical setting was 6:17 AM for the autism group (range, 3:10 AM to 8:34 AM), 6:45 AM for the developmental delay group (range, 5:46 AM to 7:51 AM), and 6:46 AM for the typical group (range, 6:03 AM to 8:36 AM). The median habitual wake time as reported on the Children's Sleep Habits Questionnaire was 7 AM for the autism group, 6:30 AM for the group with developmental delay, and 7 AM for the typical group (n = 80 after listwise deletion of Children's Sleep Habits Questionnaire nonrespondents).

Figure 1 presents differences among the 3 cohorts in age, total sleep time, stage 1 sleep percentage, and stage 3 sleep percentage graphically. Figure 2 shows the group differences in REM sleep percentage.

Place holder to copy figure label and caption
Figure 1.

Age, total sleep time, stage 1 sleep percentage, and stage 3 sleep (slow-wave sleep) percentage by group. Dark lines denote group medians; dashed lines, interquartile range. DD indicates developmental delay.

Graphic Jump Location
Place holder to copy figure label and caption
Figure 2.

Rapid eye movement (REM) sleep percentage by group. Dark lines denote group medians; dashed lines, interquartile range. DD indicates developmental delay.

Graphic Jump Location

Our data reveal autism-specific differences in sleep architecture, with the largest median differences occurring in the percentage of REM sleep attained in the autism group in comparison with both a typically developing cohort and a cohort of children with nonspecific developmental delay. This relative deficiency of REM sleep warrants further investigation. The function of REM sleep and its relationship to cognition and overall neurological health is unknown and a subject of ongoing research. We know from animal studies that REM sleep increases after intensive learning sessions,24,25 which may imply that this stage is important for particular cognitive processes and that REM sleep may be useful as an indicator of brain plasticity. Current studies continue to add support for this postulate.2629 Rapid eye movement sleep has most recently been implicated in the process of human memory consolidation and several studies suggest that it is crucial to normal cognitive function and in the processing of emotion in memory systems.30,31

Additionally, there is converging evidence that REM sleep serves a specific developmental role. The amount of time spent in REM sleep decreases over the life span. About 50% of a neonate's sleep is devoted to REM vs about 20% to 25% of adult sleep.32 The decline is the most rapid over the first few years of life and adult values are achieved before puberty.33 One hypothesis, first proposed in 1966 by Roffwarg and associates34 and continuing to be explored today, is that the increased ponto-thalamo-cortical activity evident during REM sleep provides the endogenous stimulation needed to form and stabilize durable synaptic connections in the developing brain.35,36 In support of the theory that REM sleep abnormalities early in life may have long-term consequences, Vogel et al37,38 have shown that REM sleep deprivation in the neonatal rat induces social deficiencies in the adult animal. Furthermore, in recent animal models of learning and memory exploring this question, REM sleep deficiency has been found to have differential effects on markers of neuronal plasticity (as measured by changes in molecular components of glutamatergic synapses) that are age dependent.39 For example, in work published by Lopez and colleagues in 2008, they suggest that REM sleep deprivation early in life may interfere with the correct formation of neural circuits in the hippocampus “possibly by hindering expression of mature glutamatergic synaptic components.”39(p44)

Our study has several limitations. The recordings were conducted on a regular pediatric ward and not in a specialized sleep laboratory. While the total sleep times for the typical controls are comparable with other pediatric laboratory-based studies,40 the sleep efficiency is not as great for any of the 3 groups as might have been obtained in a more controlled environment. Another limitation is that respiratory parameters were not measured, rendering it impossible to know the potential contribution of obstructive sleep apnea to sleep architecture in any of the cohorts. The prevalence of obstructive sleep apnea in the general pediatric population is estimated at 2%. This is an important consideration, but it may not have a direct impact on the interpretation of these results. While it is true that obstructive sleep apnea is more likely to occur in REM than non-REM sleep in children, sleep architecture appears to be preserved in children with obstructive sleep apnea without the associated electroencephalogram arousal and measurable sleep fragmentation that often follows obstructive events in adults.41 A third limitation is the potential effects of various medications on sleep stages. Being medication free was not an inclusion criterion for either of the parent studies. In the autism group, there was no statistically significant difference in the median REM sleep percentage among those taking any medication (11.1%) and those not (14.7%; P = .17).

Investigation of sleep-state transitions in autism offers an opportunity to speculate on the underlying neurotransmission. The control of wake/non-REM/REM rhythms is complex, involving γ-aminobutyric acid, glutamate, and 3 major neurotransmitter systems: serotonin, produced by cells in the dorsal raphe nucleus; norepinephrine, from the locus coeruleus; and acetylcholine, with neurons in the basal forebrain and the brainstem and interneurons in the striatum. Acetylcholine is the main driver of REM sleep and in concert with serotonin and norepinephrine, it acts on brainstem structures in the pons and adjacent areas of the midbrain via “REM/on” and “REM/off” cells to coordinate REM and non-REM sleep.42 It is this reciprocal model of inhibitory aminergic neurons and excitatory cholinergic neurons that modulates brain activity between the non-REM and REM states. An imbalance in either arm may result in state instability. It is our conjecture that in the developing brain the proper stabilization of synaptic pathways is at least partly dependent on the rich periods of disinhibition that occur during the REM state. Neuropathologic investigations by Bauman and Kemper43 in 1994, Perry et al44 in 2001, Lee et al45 in 2002, and Martin-Ruiz et al46 in 2004 implicate the cholinergic system in the abnormal development of the autistic brain. When considered in conjunction with the knowledge that cholinergic afferents innervate the cerebral cortex during a period of intense neuronal differentiation and synapse formation,47 the findings highlight the relevance of further investigation of the cholinergic system in autism, particularly early in development.

The differences in slow-wave sleep percentage between the autism group and both the typically developing cohort and the developmental delay group without autism may offer further clues as to whether the underlying differences in sleep architecture between cohorts are mediated by cholinergic abnormalities. During slow-wave sleep, a different neurotransmission milieu is evident than is present during REM sleep. Slow-wave appearance is greatly influenced by the presence of acetylcholine. Unlike in REM sleep where cholinergic input is high, slow-wave sleep is characterized by a relative diminution of this neurotransmitter.48,49 A more global (not just in the pontine brainstem where REM is orchestrated) cholinergic deficiency would predict a more permissive environment for slow-wave generation and could conceivably be represented by a higher percentage of this state, which was, in fact, what we found in the autism group.

In summary, the extant literature supports a far-reaching hypothesis that a primary cholinergic deficiency may simultaneously produce deficits in REM sleep in autism and contribute to the social-emotional deficits that are at the core of the autism phenotype both directly and indirectly (through the lack of appropriate developmental support provided by REM sleep early in development). Future studies should attempt to disentangle these possibilities as a means of linking the profound social and emotional processing deficits that characterize autism to a fundamental deficit in physiological regulation. Should such a link be discovered, it would provide an immediate target for pharmacologic therapies and could also lead to the development of an objective biomarker for identification of infants at risk for autism.

Correspondence: Ashura Williams Buckley, MD, National Institute of Mental Health, 10 Center Dr, Bethesda, MD 20892 (ashura.buckley@gmail.com).

Accepted for Publication: April 14, 2010.

Author Contributions: Dr Buckley acknowledges full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Study concept and design: A. W. Buckley, Thurm, and Swedo. Acquisition of data: A. W. Buckley, Rodriguez, Jennison, Thurm, Sato, and Swedo. Analysis and interpretation of data: A. W. Buckley, Rodriguez, Jennison, J. Buckley, Thurm, Sato, and Swedo. Drafting of the manuscript: A. W. Buckley, Rodriguez, Jennison, J. Buckley, and Thurm. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: A. W. Buckley, Rodriguez, J. Buckley, Thurm, Sato, and Swedo. Statistical analysis: J. Buckley and Thurm. Administrative, technical, and material support: A. W. Buckley, Rodriguez, Jennison, Thurm, and Sato. Study supervision: A. W. Buckley, Rodriguez, Thurm, Sato, and Swedo.

Financial Disclosure: None reported.

Funding/Support: All research was funded by the National Institutes of Health intramural research program.

Additional Contributions: We thank Allan Rechtschaffen, PhD, for critical insights and helpful discussions.

Rapin  I Autism. N Engl J Med 1997;337 (2) 97- 104
PubMed
Penn  HE Neurobiological correlates of autism: a review of recent research. Child Neuropsychol 2006;12 (1) 57- 79
PubMed
Johnson  KPMalow  BA Assessment and pharmacologic treatment of sleep disturbance in autism. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am 2008;17 (4) 773- 785
PubMed
Richdale  AL Sleep problems in autism: prevalence, cause, and intervention. Dev Med Child Neurol 1999;41 (1) 60- 66
PubMed
Malow  BA Sleep disorders, epilepsy, and autism. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev 2004;10 (2) 122- 125
PubMed
Patzold  LMRichdale  ALTonge  BJ An investigation into sleep characteristics of children with autism and Asperger's disorder. J Paediatr Child Health 1998;34 (6) 528- 533
PubMed
Stores  GWiggs  L Abnormal sleep patterns associated with autism: a brief review of research findings, assessment methods and treatment strategies. Autism 1998;2 (2) 157- 16910.1177/1362361398022004
Gail Williams  PSears  LLAllard  A Sleep problems in children with autism. J Sleep Res 2004;13 (3) 265- 268
PubMed
Segawa  MKatoh  MKatoh  JNomura  Y Early modulation of sleep parameters and its importance in later behavior. Brain Dysfunct 1992;5211- 223
Goodlin-Jones  BLTang  KLiu  JAnders  TF Sleep patterns in preschool-age children with autism, developmental delay, and typical development. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2008;47 (8) 930- 938
PubMed
Myers  SMJohnson  CPAmerican Academy of Pediatrics Council on Children With Disabilities, Management of children with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics 2007;120 (5) 1162- 1182
PubMed
Kryer  MHRoth  TDement  W Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine. 4th ed. Philadelphia, PA Elsevier Saunders2005;
Tanguay  PEOrnitz  EMForsythe  ABRitvo  ER Rapid eye movement (REM) activity in normal and autistic children during REM sleep. J Autism Child Schizophr 1976;6 (3) 275- 288
PubMed
Diomedi  MCuratolo  PScalise  APlacidi  FCaretto  FGigli  GL Sleep abnormalities in mentally retarded autistic subjects: Down's syndrome with mental retardation and normal subjects. Brain Dev 1999;21 (8) 548- 553
PubMed
Elia  MFerri  RMusumeci  SA  et al.  Sleep in subjects with autistic disorder: a neurophysiological and psychological study. Brain Dev 2000;22 (2) 88- 92
PubMed
Thirumalai  SSShubin  RARobinson  R Rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder in children with autism. J Child Neurol 2002;17 (3) 173- 178
PubMed
Johnson  KPGiannotti  FCortesi  F Sleep patterns in autism spectrum disorders. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am 2009;18 (4) 917- 928
PubMed
Richdale  ALSchreck  KA Sleep problems in autism spectrum disorders: prevalence, nature, & possible biopsychosocial aetiologies. Sleep Med Rev 2009;13 (6) 403- 411
PubMed
Lord  CRutter  MDiLavore  PRisi  S Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule.  Los Angeles, CA Western Psychological Services1999;
Lord  CRutter  MLe Couteur  A Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 1994;24 (5) 659- 685
PubMed
Owens  JASpirito  A McGuinn  M The Children's Sleep Habits Questionnaire (CSHQ): psychometric properties of a survey instrument for school-aged children. Sleep 2000;23 (8) 1043- 1051
PubMed
Iber  CAncoli-Israel  SChesson  AQuan  SFAmerican Academy of Sleep Medicine, The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events: Rules, Terminology and Technical Specification.  Westchester, IL American Academy of Sleep Medicine2007;
Hodges  JLLehmann  EL Estimates of location based on rank tests. Ann Math Stat 1963;34 (2) 598- 61110.1214/aoms/1177704172
McGrath  MJCohen  DB REM sleep facilitation of adaptive waking behavior: a review of the literature. Psychol Bull 1978;85 (1) 24- 57
PubMed
Mirmiran  Mvan den Dungen  HUylings  HB Sleep patterns during rearing under different environmental conditions in juvenile rats. Brain Res 1982;233 (2) 287- 298
PubMed
Guzman-Marin  RYing  ZSuntsova  N  et al.  Suppression of hippocampal plasticity-related gene expression by sleep deprivation in rats. J Physiol 2006;575 (pt 3) 807- 819
PubMed
Lee  MGHassani  OKAlonso  AJones  BE Cholinergic basal forebrain neurons burst with theta during waking and paradoxical sleep. J Neurosci 2005;25 (17) 4365- 4369
PubMed
Datta  SMavanji  VUlloor  JPatterson  EH Activation of phasic pontine-wave generator prevents rapid eye movement sleep deprivation-induced learning impairment in the rat: a mechanism for sleep-dependent plasticity. J Neurosci 2004;24 (6) 1416- 1427
PubMed
Shaffery  JPSinton  CMBissette  GRoffwarg  HPMarks  GA Rapid eye movement sleep deprivation modifies expression of long-term potentiation in visual cortex of immature rats. Neuroscience 2002;110 (3) 431- 443
PubMed
Stickgold  R Sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Nature 2005;437 (7063) 1272- 1278
PubMed
Maquet  P The role of sleep in learning and memory. Science 2001;294 (5544) 1048- 1052
PubMed
Hobson  JAPace-Schott  EF The cognitive neuroscience of sleep: neuronal systems, consciousness and learning. Nat Rev Neurosci 2002;3 (9) 679- 693
PubMed
Hobson  JA Sleep.  New York, NY Scientific American Library1989;
Roffwarg  HPMuzio  JNDement  WC Ontogenetic development of the human sleep-dream cycle. Science 1966;152 (3722) 604- 619
PubMed
Marks  GAShaffery  JPOksenberg  ASpeciale  SGRoffwarg  HP A functional role for REM sleep in brain maturation. Behav Brain Res 1995;69 (1-2) 1- 11
PubMed
Hobson  JA REM sleep and dreaming: towards a theory of protoconsciousness. Nat Rev Neurosci 2009;10 (11) 803- 813
PubMed
Vogel  GNeill  DKors  DHagler  M REM sleep abnormalities in a new animal model of endogenous depression. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1990;14 (1) 77- 83
PubMed
Vogel  GHagler  M Effects of neonatally administered iprindole on adult behaviors of rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1996;55 (1) 157- 161
PubMed
Lopez  JRoffwarg  HPDreher  ABissette  GKarolewicz  BShaffery  JP Rapid eye movement sleep deprivation decreases long-term potentiation stability and affects some glutamatergic signaling proteins during hippocampal development. Neuroscience 2008;153 (1) 44- 53
PubMed
Montgomery-Downs  HEO’Brien  LMGulliver  TEGozal  D Polysomnographic characteristics in normal preschool and early school-aged children. Pediatrics 2006;117 (3) 741- 753
PubMed
Marcus  CL Sleep-disordered breathing in children. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 2001;164 (1) 16- 30
PubMed
McCarley  RW Neurobiology of REM and NREM sleep. Sleep Med 2007;8 (4) 302- 330
PubMed
Bauman  MLKemper  TL Neuroanatomic observations of the brain in autism. Bauman  MLKemper  TLThe Neurobiology of Autism. Baltimore, MD Johns Hopkins University Press1994;
Perry  EKLee  MLMartin-Ruiz  CM  et al.  Cholinergic activity in autism: abnormalities in the cerebral cortex and basal forebrain. Am J Psychiatry 2001;158 (7) 1058- 1066
PubMed
Lee  MMartin-Ruiz  CGraham  A  et al.  Nicotinic receptor abnormalities in the cerebellar cortex in autism. Brain 2002;125 (pt 7) 1483- 1495
PubMed
Martin-Ruiz  CMLee  MPerry  RHBaumann  MCourt  JAPerry  EK Molecular analysis of nicotinic receptor expression in autism. Brain Res Mol Brain Res 2004;123 (1-2) 81- 90
PubMed
Hohmann  CFBerger-Sweeney  J Cholinergic regulation of cortical development and plasticity: new twists to an old story. Perspect Dev Neurobiol 1998;5 (4) 401- 425
PubMed
Steriade  MDatta  SParé  DOakson  GCurró Dossi  RC Neuronal activities in brain-stem cholinergic nuclei related to tonic activation processes in thalamocortical systems. J Neurosci 1990;10 (8) 2541- 2559
PubMed
Rainnie  DGGrunze  HC McCarley  RWGreene  RW Adenosine inhibition of mesopontine cholinergic neurons: implications for EEG arousal. Science 1994;263 (5147) 689- 692
PubMed

Figures

Place holder to copy figure label and caption
Figure 1.

Age, total sleep time, stage 1 sleep percentage, and stage 3 sleep (slow-wave sleep) percentage by group. Dark lines denote group medians; dashed lines, interquartile range. DD indicates developmental delay.

Graphic Jump Location
Place holder to copy figure label and caption
Figure 2.

Rapid eye movement (REM) sleep percentage by group. Dark lines denote group medians; dashed lines, interquartile range. DD indicates developmental delay.

Graphic Jump Location

Tables

Table Graphic Jump LocationTable 2. Children With Autism Have Shorter Total Sleep Time, Increased SWS, and Relative REM Sleep Deficiency

References

Rapin  I Autism. N Engl J Med 1997;337 (2) 97- 104
PubMed
Penn  HE Neurobiological correlates of autism: a review of recent research. Child Neuropsychol 2006;12 (1) 57- 79
PubMed
Johnson  KPMalow  BA Assessment and pharmacologic treatment of sleep disturbance in autism. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am 2008;17 (4) 773- 785
PubMed
Richdale  AL Sleep problems in autism: prevalence, cause, and intervention. Dev Med Child Neurol 1999;41 (1) 60- 66
PubMed
Malow  BA Sleep disorders, epilepsy, and autism. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev 2004;10 (2) 122- 125
PubMed
Patzold  LMRichdale  ALTonge  BJ An investigation into sleep characteristics of children with autism and Asperger's disorder. J Paediatr Child Health 1998;34 (6) 528- 533
PubMed
Stores  GWiggs  L Abnormal sleep patterns associated with autism: a brief review of research findings, assessment methods and treatment strategies. Autism 1998;2 (2) 157- 16910.1177/1362361398022004
Gail Williams  PSears  LLAllard  A Sleep problems in children with autism. J Sleep Res 2004;13 (3) 265- 268
PubMed
Segawa  MKatoh  MKatoh  JNomura  Y Early modulation of sleep parameters and its importance in later behavior. Brain Dysfunct 1992;5211- 223
Goodlin-Jones  BLTang  KLiu  JAnders  TF Sleep patterns in preschool-age children with autism, developmental delay, and typical development. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2008;47 (8) 930- 938
PubMed
Myers  SMJohnson  CPAmerican Academy of Pediatrics Council on Children With Disabilities, Management of children with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics 2007;120 (5) 1162- 1182
PubMed
Kryer  MHRoth  TDement  W Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine. 4th ed. Philadelphia, PA Elsevier Saunders2005;
Tanguay  PEOrnitz  EMForsythe  ABRitvo  ER Rapid eye movement (REM) activity in normal and autistic children during REM sleep. J Autism Child Schizophr 1976;6 (3) 275- 288
PubMed
Diomedi  MCuratolo  PScalise  APlacidi  FCaretto  FGigli  GL Sleep abnormalities in mentally retarded autistic subjects: Down's syndrome with mental retardation and normal subjects. Brain Dev 1999;21 (8) 548- 553
PubMed
Elia  MFerri  RMusumeci  SA  et al.  Sleep in subjects with autistic disorder: a neurophysiological and psychological study. Brain Dev 2000;22 (2) 88- 92
PubMed
Thirumalai  SSShubin  RARobinson  R Rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder in children with autism. J Child Neurol 2002;17 (3) 173- 178
PubMed
Johnson  KPGiannotti  FCortesi  F Sleep patterns in autism spectrum disorders. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am 2009;18 (4) 917- 928
PubMed
Richdale  ALSchreck  KA Sleep problems in autism spectrum disorders: prevalence, nature, & possible biopsychosocial aetiologies. Sleep Med Rev 2009;13 (6) 403- 411
PubMed
Lord  CRutter  MDiLavore  PRisi  S Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule.  Los Angeles, CA Western Psychological Services1999;
Lord  CRutter  MLe Couteur  A Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 1994;24 (5) 659- 685
PubMed
Owens  JASpirito  A McGuinn  M The Children's Sleep Habits Questionnaire (CSHQ): psychometric properties of a survey instrument for school-aged children. Sleep 2000;23 (8) 1043- 1051
PubMed
Iber  CAncoli-Israel  SChesson  AQuan  SFAmerican Academy of Sleep Medicine, The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events: Rules, Terminology and Technical Specification.  Westchester, IL American Academy of Sleep Medicine2007;
Hodges  JLLehmann  EL Estimates of location based on rank tests. Ann Math Stat 1963;34 (2) 598- 61110.1214/aoms/1177704172
McGrath  MJCohen  DB REM sleep facilitation of adaptive waking behavior: a review of the literature. Psychol Bull 1978;85 (1) 24- 57
PubMed
Mirmiran  Mvan den Dungen  HUylings  HB Sleep patterns during rearing under different environmental conditions in juvenile rats. Brain Res 1982;233 (2) 287- 298
PubMed
Guzman-Marin  RYing  ZSuntsova  N  et al.  Suppression of hippocampal plasticity-related gene expression by sleep deprivation in rats. J Physiol 2006;575 (pt 3) 807- 819
PubMed
Lee  MGHassani  OKAlonso  AJones  BE Cholinergic basal forebrain neurons burst with theta during waking and paradoxical sleep. J Neurosci 2005;25 (17) 4365- 4369
PubMed
Datta  SMavanji  VUlloor  JPatterson  EH Activation of phasic pontine-wave generator prevents rapid eye movement sleep deprivation-induced learning impairment in the rat: a mechanism for sleep-dependent plasticity. J Neurosci 2004;24 (6) 1416- 1427
PubMed
Shaffery  JPSinton  CMBissette  GRoffwarg  HPMarks  GA Rapid eye movement sleep deprivation modifies expression of long-term potentiation in visual cortex of immature rats. Neuroscience 2002;110 (3) 431- 443
PubMed
Stickgold  R Sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Nature 2005;437 (7063) 1272- 1278
PubMed
Maquet  P The role of sleep in learning and memory. Science 2001;294 (5544) 1048- 1052
PubMed
Hobson  JAPace-Schott  EF The cognitive neuroscience of sleep: neuronal systems, consciousness and learning. Nat Rev Neurosci 2002;3 (9) 679- 693
PubMed
Hobson  JA Sleep.  New York, NY Scientific American Library1989;
Roffwarg  HPMuzio  JNDement  WC Ontogenetic development of the human sleep-dream cycle. Science 1966;152 (3722) 604- 619
PubMed
Marks  GAShaffery  JPOksenberg  ASpeciale  SGRoffwarg  HP A functional role for REM sleep in brain maturation. Behav Brain Res 1995;69 (1-2) 1- 11
PubMed
Hobson  JA REM sleep and dreaming: towards a theory of protoconsciousness. Nat Rev Neurosci 2009;10 (11) 803- 813
PubMed
Vogel  GNeill  DKors  DHagler  M REM sleep abnormalities in a new animal model of endogenous depression. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1990;14 (1) 77- 83
PubMed
Vogel  GHagler  M Effects of neonatally administered iprindole on adult behaviors of rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1996;55 (1) 157- 161
PubMed
Lopez  JRoffwarg  HPDreher  ABissette  GKarolewicz  BShaffery  JP Rapid eye movement sleep deprivation decreases long-term potentiation stability and affects some glutamatergic signaling proteins during hippocampal development. Neuroscience 2008;153 (1) 44- 53
PubMed
Montgomery-Downs  HEO’Brien  LMGulliver  TEGozal  D Polysomnographic characteristics in normal preschool and early school-aged children. Pediatrics 2006;117 (3) 741- 753
PubMed
Marcus  CL Sleep-disordered breathing in children. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 2001;164 (1) 16- 30
PubMed
McCarley  RW Neurobiology of REM and NREM sleep. Sleep Med 2007;8 (4) 302- 330
PubMed
Bauman  MLKemper  TL Neuroanatomic observations of the brain in autism. Bauman  MLKemper  TLThe Neurobiology of Autism. Baltimore, MD Johns Hopkins University Press1994;
Perry  EKLee  MLMartin-Ruiz  CM  et al.  Cholinergic activity in autism: abnormalities in the cerebral cortex and basal forebrain. Am J Psychiatry 2001;158 (7) 1058- 1066
PubMed
Lee  MMartin-Ruiz  CGraham  A  et al.  Nicotinic receptor abnormalities in the cerebellar cortex in autism. Brain 2002;125 (pt 7) 1483- 1495
PubMed
Martin-Ruiz  CMLee  MPerry  RHBaumann  MCourt  JAPerry  EK Molecular analysis of nicotinic receptor expression in autism. Brain Res Mol Brain Res 2004;123 (1-2) 81- 90
PubMed
Hohmann  CFBerger-Sweeney  J Cholinergic regulation of cortical development and plasticity: new twists to an old story. Perspect Dev Neurobiol 1998;5 (4) 401- 425
PubMed
Steriade  MDatta  SParé  DOakson  GCurró Dossi  RC Neuronal activities in brain-stem cholinergic nuclei related to tonic activation processes in thalamocortical systems. J Neurosci 1990;10 (8) 2541- 2559
PubMed
Rainnie  DGGrunze  HC McCarley  RWGreene  RW Adenosine inhibition of mesopontine cholinergic neurons: implications for EEG arousal. Science 1994;263 (5147) 689- 692
PubMed

Correspondence

CME
Meets CME requirements for:
Browse CME for all U.S. States
Accreditation Information
The American Medical Association is accredited by the Accreditation Council for Continuing Medical Education to provide continuing medical education for physicians. The AMA designates this journal-based CME activity for a maximum of 1 AMA PRA Category 1 CreditTM per course. Physicians should claim only the credit commensurate with the extent of their participation in the activity. Physicians who complete the CME course and score at least 80% correct on the quiz are eligible for AMA PRA Category 1 CreditTM.
Note: You must get at least of the answers correct to pass this quiz.
You have not filled in all the answers to complete this quiz
The following questions were not answered:
Sorry, you have unsuccessfully completed this CME quiz with a score of
The following questions were not answered correctly:
Commitment to Change (optional):
Indicate what change(s) you will implement in your practice, if any, based on this CME course.
Your quiz results:
The filled radio buttons indicate your responses. The preferred responses are highlighted
For CME Course: A Proposed Model for Initial Assessment and Management of Acute Heart Failure Syndromes
Indicate what changes(s) you will implement in your practice, if any, based on this CME course.
NOTE:
Citing articles are presented as examples only. In non-demo SCM6 implementation, integration with CrossRef’s "Cited By" API will populate this tab (http://www.crossref.org/citedby.html).
Submit a Comment

Multimedia

Some tools below are only available to our subscribers or users with an online account.

Related Content

Customize your page view by dragging & repositioning the boxes below.

Articles Related By Topic
Related Topics
PubMed Articles