0
We're unable to sign you in at this time. Please try again in a few minutes.
Retry
We were able to sign you in, but your subscription(s) could not be found. Please try again in a few minutes.
Retry
There may be a problem with your account. Please contact the AMA Service Center to resolve this issue.
Contact the AMA Service Center:
Telephone: 1 (800) 262-2350 or 1 (312) 670-7827  *   Email: subscriptions@jamanetwork.com
Error Message ......
Article |

Short-term and Long-term Effects of Violent Media on Aggression in Children and Adults FREE

Brad J. Bushman, PhD; L. Rowell Huesmann, PhD
[+] Author Affiliations

Author Affiliations: Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, the Netherlands (Dr Bushman); and Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor (Drs Bushman and Huesmann).


Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2006;160(4):348-352. doi:10.1001/archpedi.160.4.348.
Text Size: A A A
Published online

Objectives  To test whether the results of the accumulated studies on media violence and aggressive behavior are consistent with the theories that have evolved to explain the effects. We tested for the existence of both short-term and long-term effects for aggressive behavior. We also tested the theory-driven hypothesis that short-term effects should be greater for adults and long-term effects should be greater for children.

Design  Meta-analysis.

Participants  Children younger than 18 years and adults.

Main Exposures  Violent media, including TV, movies, video games, music, and comic books.

Main Outcome Measures  Measures of aggressive behavior, aggressive thoughts, angry feelings, physiological arousal (eg, heart rate, blood pressure), and helping behavior.

Results  Effect size estimates were combined using meta-analytic procedures. As expected, the short-term effects of violent media were greater for adults than for children whereas the long-term effects were greater for children than for adults. The results also showed that there were overall modest but significant effect sizes for exposure to media violence on aggressive behaviors, aggressive thoughts, angry feelings, arousal levels, and helping behavior.

Conclusions  The results are consistent with the theory that short-term effects are mostly due to the priming of existing well-encoded scripts, schemas, or beliefs, which adults have had more time to encode. In contrast, long-term effects require the learning (encoding) of scripts, schemas, or beliefs. Children can encode new scripts, schemas, and beliefs via observational learning with less interference and effort than adults.

Figures in this Article

The body of empirical research linking children's exposure to media violence with subsequent increases in their aggressive and violent behavior was already substantial by the 1970s. The 1972 Surgeon General's Scientific Advisory Committee on Television and Social Behavior report1 and the National Institute of Mental Health 10-year follow-up report2 provided widely accessible summaries of this growing body of research. By the 1980s, most child development scholars had accepted the theory that exposure to media violence, at least during some periods of a child's development, increased their risk for aggression.3 However, it has only been in the 1990s and more recently that meta-analyses have provided systematic summaries of the entire body of research.4,5 Similarly, it has been during this time that the attention of researchers has turned more toward investigating the processes producing the effects and developing a more coherent theoretical understanding of the effects.6

In this article, we describe what we believe are the major psychological processes that account for the relationship between children's exposure to different kinds of violence in their world and the increase in the risk for violence and aggression that this exposure produces. We then use meta-analyses to show that, on the whole, the available empirical data show the patterns one would expect from this theory. Although the focus of this article is on exposure to media violence, the theoretical premise is that the same processes operate when children are exposed to media violence as when they are exposed to violence on the street, in the home, or among their peers.

The psychological processes that link children's exposure to violence with subsequent increases in children's aggressive behaviors can be divided into those that produce more immediate but transient short-term changes in behavior and those that produce more delayed but enduring long-term changes in behavior. Long-term increases in children's aggressive behavior are now generally agreed to be a consequence of the child's learning scripts for aggressive behavior, cognitions supporting aggression, and aggression-promoting emotions through the observation of others behaving violently. This observational learning generally requires the repeated observation of violence. On the other hand, short-term increases in children's aggressive behavior following the observation of violence are owing to 3 other quite different psychological processes: (1) the priming of already existing aggressive behavioral scripts, aggressive cognitions, or angry emotional reactions; (2) simple mimicking of aggressive scripts; and (3) changes in emotional arousal stimulated by the observation of violence.

Neuroscientists and cognitive psychologists posit that the human mind acts as an associative network in which ideas are partially activated, or primed, by stimuli that they are associated with.7 The activation produced by an observed stimulus spreads in the network and moves even remotely related concepts closer to a threshold of influence. Thus, an encounter with an event or object can prime related concepts, ideas, and emotions in a person's memory, even without the person being aware of it.8 For example, the mere presence of a weapon in a person's visual field can increase aggressive thoughts or behaviors.9 Seeing people fight activates scripts for behavior related to fighting and other aggressive ideas. Any cognitions, behaviors, or emotions that have ever been linked to an observed violent scene will be activated within milliseconds when that scene is observed.

Human and primate young have an innate tendency to imitate whomever they observe.10,11 Neuroscientists have discovered “mirror neurons” in primates that seem to promote such processing.12 Consequently, children who observe (in the media or in the environment around them) others exhibiting a specific aggressive behavior, eg, hitting, are more likely to perform the same aggressive behavior immediately.11,13,14 Theoretically, the more similar that children think they and the observed model are, the more readily imitation will take place, but the imitation mechanism is so powerful that even fantasy characters are imitated by young children.

Observed violence often consists of high-action sequences that are very arousing for youth as measured by increased heart rate, blood pressure, skin conductance of electricity, and other physiological indices of arousal. To the extent that media violence highly arouses the observer, aggressive behaviors may become more likely in the short run for 2 possible reasons. First, high arousal generated by exposure to violence makes any dominant response tendency more likely to be carried out in the short term. Consequently, the child with aggressive tendencies behaves even more aggressively.15,16 Second, when a child is highly aroused (eg, by viewing violence), a mild specific emotion (eg, mild anger) experienced some time later may be felt more severely (eg, intense anger) than otherwise because some of the emotional response stimulated by the violent media presentation is misattributed as being due to the provocation. This process is called excitation transfer.17,18

By observational learning, we mean the process through which behavioral scripts, world schemas, and normative beliefs become encoded in a child's mind simply as a consequence of the child observing others. Observational learning is a powerful extension of imitation in which logical induction and abstraction are used to encode complex representations. For example, extensive observation of violence biases children's world schemas toward hostility, and they then attribute more hostility to others' actions,19,20 which in turn increases the likelihood of children behaving aggressively themselves.21 Similarly, through repeated observation of real-life aggressive models and aggressive models portrayed in the media, children develop normative beliefs that aggression is appropriate, and they acquire social scripts for how to behave aggressively. Whereas short-term mimicry requires only 1 exposure to an observed behavior, long-term observational learning usually (but not always) requires repeated exposures. The more that the child's attention is riveted on the observed behavior, the fewer are the number of repetitions needed. However, numerous other factors besides attention affect the extent of the learning. The current conceptions of this process have grown out of the convergence of social learning theory13 with more recent theories of social information processing.19,2225 The more that the child identifies with the observed people and the more that the observed scripts for behavior are rewarded and portrayed as appropriate, the more firmly will the scripts be encoded and the more likely it is that more general beliefs about such behaviors will be extracted and encoded.13,26

This observational learning interacts with conditioning by family and peers to build behavioral scripts and social cognitions that are highly resistant to change. The reinforcements that a child receives from imitating a positive or negative behavior strongly influence the likelihood of that behavior persisting.15,26 Similarly, if the world schemas and normative beliefs that a child acquires through observing others (again, in real life and in the media) lead to valuable outcomes for the child, they will become more firmly encoded and more resistant to change.27

The long-term effects that exposure to violence has on children also involve the vicarious conditioning of emotional reactions. Through classical conditioning, fear or anger can become linked with specific stimuli after only a few exposures.2830 These emotions influence behavior in social settings away from the media source through stimulus generalization. A child may then react with inappropriate fear or anger in a novel situation that is similar to one that the child has observed in the media. Repeated exposure to emotionally arousing media can also lead to habituation of certain natural emotional reactions. This process is often called desensitization, and it has been used to explain a reduction in distress-related physiological reactivity to media portrayals of violence. Indeed, violent scenes do become less arousing over time,31 and brief exposure to media violence can reduce physiological reactions to real-world violence.32 Behaviors that might seem unusual to the child viewer at first will begin to seem more normative after repeated presentations. For example, most persons seem to have an innate negative emotional response to observing blood and violence as evidenced by increased heart rates, perspiration, and self-reports of discomfort that often accompany such exposure. However, with repeated exposure, this negative emotional response habituates, and the child becomes desensitized. The child can then think about and plan proactive aggressive acts without experiencing negative affect.

For both adults and children, we expect that there will be positive relationships between their degree of exposure to media violence and their subsequent short-term displays of aggressive behaviors, emotions, and ideas. Priming, imitation, and excitation transfer would all contribute to these effects.

These short-term effects should be more pronounced for adults than for children because priming depends on the prior existence of a well-encoded network of aggressive scripts, beliefs, and schemas. The development and elaboration of such networks require time and repeated learning experiences, so adults are more likely to possess well-elaborated, rich networks of associations involving aggressive scripts, beliefs, and schemas. Consequently, observations of violence by adults will prime a set of related aggressive constructs larger than that for children and will prime them more rapidly.

There should be positive relationships for both adults and children between their degree of exposure to media violence earlier in life and their subsequent long-term displays of aggressive scripts, beliefs, and schemas. Observational learning of scripts, schemas, and beliefs and the desensitization of negative emotional reactions to violence are the 2 processes that contribute to these effects.

These long-term effects should be more pronounced for children than for adults because observational learning and desensitization both depend on the observer having a mind that is readily modifiable. The younger the children are, the more susceptible they should be to encoding aggressive scripts, schemas, or beliefs through observational learning; also, the younger the children are, the more quickly their emotional responses to observing violence should habituate. For adults, learning new scripts, schemas, and beliefs requires replacing old ones, and the process is likely to take longer and require the observation of more powerful scenes. In the rest of this article, we examine whether meta-analyses of the accumulated empirical data on media violence are consistent with these predictions.

DATA SOURCES

To retrieve relevant studies, we searched the PsychINFO database from 1887 (starting date) to 2000 using the terms violen* or aggress* as well as the terms TV, televis*, film, movie, screen, music, radio, video, video game, computer game, cartoon, comic, pornograph*, erotic*, news, book, magazine, or sport. The asterisk is a wild card that gives all of the possible forms of the word (eg, aggress* searches for the terms aggress, aggressed, aggression, aggressive, aggressively, and aggressor). We restricted the search to empirical studies involving human participants. We also searched the reference sections of previous meta-analyses of violent media and aggression.4,3335

STUDY SELECTION

Two inclusion criteria were used. First, the study needed to include a measure or manipulation of violent media exposure (eg, TV programs, films, video games, music, and comic books). Second, the study needed to include a measure of aggressive behavior, aggressive cognition, aggressive affect, physiological arousal, or helping behavior. A total of 431 studies involving 68 463 participants met these criteria. There were 264 studies involving 50 312 children and 167 studies involving 18 151 adults.

CODED CHARACTERISTICS

The following characteristics were coded for each study: (1) type of study (ie, longitudinal study, laboratory experiment, field experiment, correlational study); (2) participant age (ie, children aged <18 years, adults); and (3) type of dependent variable (ie, aggressive behavior, aggressive thoughts, angry feelings, physiological arousal, helping behavior).

Each article was coded by one of us (B.J.B.). To assess coding reliability, an additional 3 judges each coded a random sample of 50 articles. A different random sample (with replacement) was selected for each judge. There was perfect agreement among judges on coded variables.

META-ANALYTIC APPROACH

The effect size index was the correlation coefficient. Because the distribution of the correlation coefficient is not normal unless the population correlation coefficient equals 0, we applied Fisher z transformation to each correlation coefficient before pooling them. Each z score was weighted by the inverse of its variance (ie, N−3). Thus, larger studies had more weight computing the average correlation. Then, we transformed z scores back to correlations. We also computed a 95% confidence interval for the average correlation. If the 95% confidence interval excludes the value 0, the correlation is significantly different from 0.

We used both fixed- and random-effects analyses, although the 2 approaches yielded the same pattern of results. Thus, we report only the fixed-effects analyses in this article.

In a meta-analysis, it is difficult to find unpublished studies. Studies with nonsignificant effects are often not published; they end up in file drawers rather than in peer-reviewed journals. If studies in file drawers had been published, the average correlation would be smaller. “Fail-safe N” is the number of studies with nonsignificant correlations that are needed to reduce the average correlation to 0.36 If the fail-safe N is large, then the results are probably not affected by publication bias.

OVERALL EFFECTS OF MEDIA VIOLENCE

As shown in the Table, there were significant, if modest, overall positive effect sizes showing that exposure to media violence was positively related to subsequent aggressive behavior, aggressive ideas, arousal, and anger. Additionally, there was a significant negative effect of exposure to violence on subsequent helping behavior. Because the confidence intervals exclude the value 0, the average correlations were all significantly different from 0. Also, the fail-safe N values are quite large, indicating that the file drawer problem is not that serious.

Table Graphic Jump LocationTable. Effects of Media Violence on Aggressive and Helpful Behavior, Aggressive Thoughts, Angry Feelings, and Physiological Arousal Levels for All of the Studies Combined*
AGE DIFFERENCES IN SHORT-TERM AND LONG-TERM VIOLENT MEDIA EFFECTS

We expected the short-term effects of violent media on aggression to be larger for adults than for children. In contrast, we expected the long-term effects of violent media on aggression to be larger for children than for adults. This pattern of results would be supported by a significant interaction between the type of study and age on violent media–related effect size estimates. We tested this hypothesis using the Statistical Analysis System (SAS Institute, Inc, Cary, NC) general linear model procedure.37 Age was a dichotomous variable in the model (ie, child vs adult). Study type was also dichotomous (laboratory experiment vs longitudinal study). The analysis revealed no main effect for age (χ2 = 0.21, N = 166, P>.05). There was a main effect for type of study (χ2 = 14.25, N = 166, P<.001), but it was qualified by the predicted interaction between age and study type (χ2 = 6.87, N = 166, P<.009). As can be seen in the Figure, media-related aggression was greater for adults than for children in laboratory studies, but it was greater for children than for adults in longitudinal studies.

Place holder to copy figure label and caption
Figure.

Interactive effects of age and study type on media-related aggression. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals.

Graphic Jump Location

In summary, none of our theoretical predictions about the relationship between exposure to media violence and subsequent aggression were contradicted by the meta-analytic data. As predicted, adults displayed larger effect sizes than children in short-term studies whereas children displayed larger effect sizes than adults in long-term studies. This is consistent with the theory that short-term effects are mostly due to the priming of existing well-encoded scripts, schemas, or beliefs whereas long-term effects require the learning (encoding) of scripts, schemas, or beliefs. Young minds with fewer existing encoded cognitions can encode new scripts, schemas, and beliefs via observational learning with less interference and effort than adults. Additionally, we showed that there were overall modest but significant effect sizes for exposure to media violence on aggressive behaviors, emotions, and cognitions.

There are important implications to be drawn from these conclusions for clinical practice. Although observing violence may increase aggression in the short term for adults and children, long-term effects are most likely to occur for children. Consequently, children need the most protection from repeated exposures to violence. Infrequent exposure is not likely to produce lasting consequences, but parents particularly need to be urged to protect their children against the kinds of repeated exposures that heavy play with violent video games or immersion in violent TV programs is likely to produce.

Parents also need to realize that the size of the long-term effect that such exposure has on children will depend on the extent to which the child perceives the violence as realistic, justified, and rewarded as well as on the extent to which the child identifies with the perpetrator. Action heroes are more dangerous teachers of violent behavior than villains. Finally, parents need to be as concerned about the beliefs and attitudes that are being conveyed in violent shows as they are about their child mimicking the behaviors shown. The changes in how the child perceives the world from viewing violence and the beliefs about aggression that the child acquires from viewing violence are likely to influence the child's behavior in the long term as much as the specific scripts for aggression that the child learns from viewing violence.

Correspondence: Brad J. Bushman, PhD, Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan, 426 Thompson St, Ann Arbor, MI 48106 (bbushman@umich.edu).

Accepted for Publication: November 30, 2005.

Surgeon General's Scientific Advisory Committee on Television and Social Behavior, Television and Growing Up: The Impact of Televised Violence.  Rockville, Md National Institute of Mental Health1972;
Pearl  DedBouthilet  LedLazar  Jed Television and Behavior: Ten Years of Scientific Progress and Implications for the Eighties.  Rockville, Md US Dept of Health and Human Services, Public Health Service, Alcohol, Drug Abuse, and Mental Health Administration, National Institute of Mental Health1982;
Murray  JP Results of an informal poll of knowledgeable persons concerning the impact of television violence Newsl Am Psychol Assoc Div Child Youth Fam Serv 1984;72- 3
Paik  HComstock  G The effects of television violence on antisocial behavior: a meta-analysis Communic Res 1994;21516- 546
Link to Article
Anderson  CABushman  BJ Media violence and societal violence Science 2002;2952377- 2378
PubMed Link to Article
Huesmann  LRMoise-Titus  JPodolski  CEron  L Longitudinal relations between children's exposure to TV violence and their aggressive and violent behavior in young adulthood: 1977-1992 Dev Psychol 2003;39201- 221
PubMed Link to Article
Fiske  STTaylor  SE Social Cognition.  Reading, Mass Addison-Wesley1984;
Bargh  JAPietromonaco  P Automatic information processing and social perception: the influence of trait information presented outside of conscious awareness on impression formation J Pers Soc Psychol 1982;43437- 449
Link to Article
Berkowitz  LLePage  A Weapons as aggression-eliciting stimuli J Pers Soc Psychol 1967;7202- 207
Link to Article
Hurley  SChatter  N Perspectives on Imitation: From Cognitive Neuroscience to Social Science.  Cambridge, Mass MIT Press2004;
Meltzoff  ANMoore  MKMuir  DedSlater  Aed Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates: resolving the debate about early imitation Infant Development: The Essential Readings. Malden, Mass Blackwell Publishers2000;167- 181
Rizzolatti  GFadiga  LGallese  VFogassi  L Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 1996;3131- 141
PubMed Link to Article
Bandura  A Social Learning Theory.  New York, NY Prentice-Hall1997;
Bandura  ARoss  DRoss  SA Imitation of film-mediated aggressive models J Abnorm Soc Psychol 1963;663- 11
PubMed Link to Article
Berkowitz  L Aggression: Its Causes, Consequences, and Control.  Boston, Mass McGraw-Hill1993;
Geen  RGO'Neal  EC Activation of cue-elicited aggression by general arousal J Pers Soc Psychol 1969;11289- 292
PubMed Link to Article
Bryant  JZillmann  D Effect of intensification of annoyance through unrelated residual excitation on substantially delayed hostile behavior J Exp Soc Psychol 1979;15470- 480
Link to Article
Zillmann  DBryant  JComisky  PW Excitation and hedonic valence in the effect of erotica on motivated inter-male aggression Eur J Soc Psychol 1981;11233- 252
Link to Article
Dodge  KAKendall  PCed Attributional bias in aggressive children Advances in Cognitive-Behavioral Research and Therapy. Vol 4 San Diego, Calif Academic Press1985;73- 110
Gerbner  GGross  LMorgan  MSignorielli  NBryant  JedZillmann  Ded Growing up with television: the cultivation perspective Media Effects. Hillsdale, NJ Erlbaum1994;17- 41
Dodge  KAPettit  GSBates  JE Social information-processing patterns partially mediate the effect of early physical abuse on later conduct problems J Abnorm Psychol 1995;104632- 643
PubMed Link to Article
Anderson  CAHuesmann  LRHogg  MAedCooper  Jed Human aggression: a social-cognitive view Sage Handbook of Social Psychology. London, England Sage2003;296- 323
Anderson  CABushman  BJ Human aggression Annu Rev Psychol 2002;5327- 51
PubMed Link to Article
Huesmann  LR An information processing model for the development of aggression Aggress Behav 1988;1413- 24
Link to Article
Huesmann  LRGeen  RGedDonnerstein  Eed The role of social information processing and cognitive schema in the acquisition and maintenance of habitual aggressive behavior Human Aggression: Theories, Research, and Implications for Social Policy. San Diego, Calif Academic Press1998;73- 109
Bandura  A Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory.  Upper Saddle River, NJ Prentice-Hall1986;
Huesmann  LRGuerra  N Children's normative beliefs about aggression and aggressive behavior J Pers Soc Psychol 1997;72408- 419
PubMed Link to Article
Cantor  JZillmann  DedBryant  Jed Confronting children's fright responses to mass media Media, Children, and the Family: Social Scientific, Psychodynamic, and Clinical Perspectives. Hillsdale, NJ Lawrence Erlbaum Associates1994;139- 150
Cantor  JReilly  S Adolescents' fright reactions to television and films J Commun 1982;3287- 89
PubMed Link to Article
Harrison  KCantor  J Tales from the screen: enduring fright reactions to scary media Media Psychol 1999;197- 116
Link to Article
Cline  VBCroft  RGCourrier  S Desensitization of children to television violence J Pers Soc Psychol 1973;27360- 365
PubMed Link to Article
Thomas  MHHorton  RWLippincott  ECDrabman  RS Desensitization to portrayals of real life aggression as a function of television violence J Pers Soc Psychol 1977;35450- 458
PubMed Link to Article
Hearold  SComstock  Ged A synthesis of 1043 effects of television on social behavior Public Communication and Behavior. Vol 1 New York, NY Academic Press1986;65- 133
Hogben  M Factors moderating the effect of television aggression on viewer behavior Communic Res 1998;25220- 247
Link to Article
Wood  WWong  FYChachere  JG Effects of media violence on viewers' aggression in unconstrained social interaction Psychol Bull 1991;109371- 383
PubMed Link to Article
Rosenthal  R Meta-analytic Procedures for Social Research.  Newbury Park, Calif Sage1991;
Wang  MCBushman  BJ Integrating Results Through Meta-analytic Review Using SAS Software.  Cary, NC SAS Institute Inc1999;

Figures

Place holder to copy figure label and caption
Figure.

Interactive effects of age and study type on media-related aggression. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals.

Graphic Jump Location

Tables

Table Graphic Jump LocationTable. Effects of Media Violence on Aggressive and Helpful Behavior, Aggressive Thoughts, Angry Feelings, and Physiological Arousal Levels for All of the Studies Combined*

References

Surgeon General's Scientific Advisory Committee on Television and Social Behavior, Television and Growing Up: The Impact of Televised Violence.  Rockville, Md National Institute of Mental Health1972;
Pearl  DedBouthilet  LedLazar  Jed Television and Behavior: Ten Years of Scientific Progress and Implications for the Eighties.  Rockville, Md US Dept of Health and Human Services, Public Health Service, Alcohol, Drug Abuse, and Mental Health Administration, National Institute of Mental Health1982;
Murray  JP Results of an informal poll of knowledgeable persons concerning the impact of television violence Newsl Am Psychol Assoc Div Child Youth Fam Serv 1984;72- 3
Paik  HComstock  G The effects of television violence on antisocial behavior: a meta-analysis Communic Res 1994;21516- 546
Link to Article
Anderson  CABushman  BJ Media violence and societal violence Science 2002;2952377- 2378
PubMed Link to Article
Huesmann  LRMoise-Titus  JPodolski  CEron  L Longitudinal relations between children's exposure to TV violence and their aggressive and violent behavior in young adulthood: 1977-1992 Dev Psychol 2003;39201- 221
PubMed Link to Article
Fiske  STTaylor  SE Social Cognition.  Reading, Mass Addison-Wesley1984;
Bargh  JAPietromonaco  P Automatic information processing and social perception: the influence of trait information presented outside of conscious awareness on impression formation J Pers Soc Psychol 1982;43437- 449
Link to Article
Berkowitz  LLePage  A Weapons as aggression-eliciting stimuli J Pers Soc Psychol 1967;7202- 207
Link to Article
Hurley  SChatter  N Perspectives on Imitation: From Cognitive Neuroscience to Social Science.  Cambridge, Mass MIT Press2004;
Meltzoff  ANMoore  MKMuir  DedSlater  Aed Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates: resolving the debate about early imitation Infant Development: The Essential Readings. Malden, Mass Blackwell Publishers2000;167- 181
Rizzolatti  GFadiga  LGallese  VFogassi  L Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 1996;3131- 141
PubMed Link to Article
Bandura  A Social Learning Theory.  New York, NY Prentice-Hall1997;
Bandura  ARoss  DRoss  SA Imitation of film-mediated aggressive models J Abnorm Soc Psychol 1963;663- 11
PubMed Link to Article
Berkowitz  L Aggression: Its Causes, Consequences, and Control.  Boston, Mass McGraw-Hill1993;
Geen  RGO'Neal  EC Activation of cue-elicited aggression by general arousal J Pers Soc Psychol 1969;11289- 292
PubMed Link to Article
Bryant  JZillmann  D Effect of intensification of annoyance through unrelated residual excitation on substantially delayed hostile behavior J Exp Soc Psychol 1979;15470- 480
Link to Article
Zillmann  DBryant  JComisky  PW Excitation and hedonic valence in the effect of erotica on motivated inter-male aggression Eur J Soc Psychol 1981;11233- 252
Link to Article
Dodge  KAKendall  PCed Attributional bias in aggressive children Advances in Cognitive-Behavioral Research and Therapy. Vol 4 San Diego, Calif Academic Press1985;73- 110
Gerbner  GGross  LMorgan  MSignorielli  NBryant  JedZillmann  Ded Growing up with television: the cultivation perspective Media Effects. Hillsdale, NJ Erlbaum1994;17- 41
Dodge  KAPettit  GSBates  JE Social information-processing patterns partially mediate the effect of early physical abuse on later conduct problems J Abnorm Psychol 1995;104632- 643
PubMed Link to Article
Anderson  CAHuesmann  LRHogg  MAedCooper  Jed Human aggression: a social-cognitive view Sage Handbook of Social Psychology. London, England Sage2003;296- 323
Anderson  CABushman  BJ Human aggression Annu Rev Psychol 2002;5327- 51
PubMed Link to Article
Huesmann  LR An information processing model for the development of aggression Aggress Behav 1988;1413- 24
Link to Article
Huesmann  LRGeen  RGedDonnerstein  Eed The role of social information processing and cognitive schema in the acquisition and maintenance of habitual aggressive behavior Human Aggression: Theories, Research, and Implications for Social Policy. San Diego, Calif Academic Press1998;73- 109
Bandura  A Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory.  Upper Saddle River, NJ Prentice-Hall1986;
Huesmann  LRGuerra  N Children's normative beliefs about aggression and aggressive behavior J Pers Soc Psychol 1997;72408- 419
PubMed Link to Article
Cantor  JZillmann  DedBryant  Jed Confronting children's fright responses to mass media Media, Children, and the Family: Social Scientific, Psychodynamic, and Clinical Perspectives. Hillsdale, NJ Lawrence Erlbaum Associates1994;139- 150
Cantor  JReilly  S Adolescents' fright reactions to television and films J Commun 1982;3287- 89
PubMed Link to Article
Harrison  KCantor  J Tales from the screen: enduring fright reactions to scary media Media Psychol 1999;197- 116
Link to Article
Cline  VBCroft  RGCourrier  S Desensitization of children to television violence J Pers Soc Psychol 1973;27360- 365
PubMed Link to Article
Thomas  MHHorton  RWLippincott  ECDrabman  RS Desensitization to portrayals of real life aggression as a function of television violence J Pers Soc Psychol 1977;35450- 458
PubMed Link to Article
Hearold  SComstock  Ged A synthesis of 1043 effects of television on social behavior Public Communication and Behavior. Vol 1 New York, NY Academic Press1986;65- 133
Hogben  M Factors moderating the effect of television aggression on viewer behavior Communic Res 1998;25220- 247
Link to Article
Wood  WWong  FYChachere  JG Effects of media violence on viewers' aggression in unconstrained social interaction Psychol Bull 1991;109371- 383
PubMed Link to Article
Rosenthal  R Meta-analytic Procedures for Social Research.  Newbury Park, Calif Sage1991;
Wang  MCBushman  BJ Integrating Results Through Meta-analytic Review Using SAS Software.  Cary, NC SAS Institute Inc1999;

Correspondence

CME
Meets CME requirements for:
Browse CME for all U.S. States
Accreditation Information
The American Medical Association is accredited by the Accreditation Council for Continuing Medical Education to provide continuing medical education for physicians. The AMA designates this journal-based CME activity for a maximum of 1 AMA PRA Category 1 CreditTM per course. Physicians should claim only the credit commensurate with the extent of their participation in the activity. Physicians who complete the CME course and score at least 80% correct on the quiz are eligible for AMA PRA Category 1 CreditTM.
Note: You must get at least of the answers correct to pass this quiz.
You have not filled in all the answers to complete this quiz
The following questions were not answered:
Sorry, you have unsuccessfully completed this CME quiz with a score of
The following questions were not answered correctly:
Commitment to Change (optional):
Indicate what change(s) you will implement in your practice, if any, based on this CME course.
Your quiz results:
The filled radio buttons indicate your responses. The preferred responses are highlighted
For CME Course: A Proposed Model for Initial Assessment and Management of Acute Heart Failure Syndromes
Indicate what changes(s) you will implement in your practice, if any, based on this CME course.
Submit a Comment

Multimedia

Some tools below are only available to our subscribers or users with an online account.

Web of Science® Times Cited: 117

Related Content

Customize your page view by dragging & repositioning the boxes below.

Articles Related By Topic
Related Collections
PubMed Articles